Close Up (Mar-Dec 1931)

Record Details:

Something wrong or inaccurate about this page? Let us Know!

Thanks for helping us continually improve the quality of the Lantern search engine for all of our users! We have millions of scanned pages, so user reports are incredibly helpful for us to identify places where we can improve and update the metadata.

Please describe the issue below, and click "Submit" to send your comments to our team! If you'd prefer, you can also send us an email to mhdl@commarts.wisc.edu with your comments.




We use Optical Character Recognition (OCR) during our scanning and processing workflow to make the content of each page searchable. You can view the automatically generated text below as well as copy and paste individual pieces of text to quote in your own work.

Text recognition is never 100% accurate. Many parts of the scanned page may not be reflected in the OCR text output, including: images, page layout, certain fonts or handwriting.

CLOSE UP Let us examine the arguments that have brought different authors from different sides and specialities, to the same, unanimously acclaimed . . . wrong suggestion. The principle arguments are four : Two from the dominion of aesthetics. One physiological. And one commercial. Let us demolish them in the order quoted. The two aesthetic arguments in favour of the horizontal shape of the screen are based on deductions deriving from traditions in the art forms of painting and stage practice. As such they should be eliminated from discussion even without being taken into consideration for the greatest errors invariably arise from the attempt to transplant practical results based upon the resemblance of the superficial appearances of one branch of art to those of another. (An entirely different practice is the discovery of similarity in methods and principles of different arts corresponding to the psychological phenomena identical and basic for all art perceptions — but the present superficially exposed analogies, as we shall see, are far from this !) Indeed, from the methodological similarity of different arts it is our task to seek out the strictest differentiation in adapting and handling them accord* ing to the organic specifics typical for each. To enforce adoption of the laws organic to one art upon another is profoundlv wrong. This practice has something of adultery in it. Like sleeping in another person's wife's bed. . . But in this case the arguments in themselves bring so mistaken a suggestion from their own proper dominion that it is worth while considering them to demonstrate their falsity. 1. Lloyd A. Jones (No 9 on the list) discusses the various rectangular proportions employed in artistic composition and gives the result of a statistical study of the proportions of paintings. The results of his research seem to favour a ratio of base to altitude considerably larger than 1, and probably over 1.5. A statement startling in itself. I don't repudiate the enormous statistical luggage that was doubtless at the disposal of Mr. Jones in enabling him to make so decisive a statement. But as I set about summoning up my pictorial recollections gathered through all the museums that I have so lately visited during my rush through Europe and America, and recalling the heaps of graphic works and compositions studied during my work, it seems to me that there are exactly as many upright standing pictures as pictures disposed in horizontal line. And everyone will agree with me. The statistical paradox of Mr. Jones derives probably from an undue weight placed upon compositional proportions of the 19th century preimpressionistic period— the worst period of painting — the " narrative " type of picture. Those second and third rate paintings, right off the progressive