Sound motion pictures : from the laboratory to their presentation (1929)

Record Details:

Something wrong or inaccurate about this page? Let us Know!

Thanks for helping us continually improve the quality of the Lantern search engine for all of our users! We have millions of scanned pages, so user reports are incredibly helpful for us to identify places where we can improve and update the metadata.

Please describe the issue below, and click "Submit" to send your comments to our team! If you'd prefer, you can also send us an email to mhdl@commarts.wisc.edu with your comments.




We use Optical Character Recognition (OCR) during our scanning and processing workflow to make the content of each page searchable. You can view the automatically generated text below as well as copy and paste individual pieces of text to quote in your own work.

Text recognition is never 100% accurate. Many parts of the scanned page may not be reflected in the OCR text output, including: images, page layout, certain fonts or handwriting.

SPEECH, MUSIC, AND HEARING 253 mental. Through the analytical investigations of sound by this method the ideal musical tone for any voice or instrument can be accurately defined and reproduced. Sound travels only 1,089 feet a second in air at 32 degrees F., the speed increasing a little more than one foot a second for every degree of rise in temperature. We see distant lightning long before we hear the peal of thunder, because light travels so much faster than sound (186,000 miles a second). Sound grows fainter with distance. We can easily understand why this is so: as the sound waves move out from the centre, the original impetus given them is spread out thinner and thinner. A locomotive whistle rises to a shrill shriek as it approaches and dies away to a low, mournful wail as the train departs, even though the whistle is emitting vibrations at the same rate all the time. The distance of sound is judged by the feebleness of its impulse on the ear. We can readily judge direction in sound, as to whence a voice proceeds — before or behind — or to the right or left of us — or above — or below us — but if we were to stand blindfold opposite a row of persons at a distance of, say, ten feet we would not be able to say which person emitted a given sound. The influence of familiarity is well exemplified in sound. We can more clearly hear a language we understand than we can a foreign language. It is comparatively easy to catch an utterance in a familiar language, even when very faintly spoken, and in the same manner a familiar voice is more easily discernible than the voice of a stranger. The better acquainted the mental system is with an impression of sound, the more easily is it heard. Thus, a familiar sound or musical composition is immediately recognized, even though heard or played very lightly, whereas an unfamiliar sound or musical composition cannot make the same impression.